Skip to main content
  • Primary research
  • Open access
  • Published:

Dysfunction of orbitofrontal and dorsolateral prefrontal cortices in children and adolescents with high-functioning pervasive developmental disorders

Abstract

Background

Several lines of evidence suggest that dysfunction of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) and orbitofrontal cortex (OFC) contributes to the pathophysiology of pervasive developmental disorders (PDD). The purpose of this study was to investigate neuropsychological dysfunctions in both the DLPFC and OFC of children and adolescents with high-functioning PDD.

Methods

The Iowa gambling task (IGT), which reflects OFC function, and the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST), which reflects DLPFC function, were assigned to 19 children and early adolescents with high-functioning PDD and 19 healthy controls matched for gender, age, and intelligence.

Results

Compared to healthy controls, patients with high-functioning PDD displayed poorer performance on the IGT and the WCST.

Conclusions

These results indicate that both the DLPFC and OFC could be impaired in children and early adolescents with high-functioning PDD.

Background

Pervasive developmental disorders (PDDs) are a group of mental disorders that are characterized by multiple developmental delays in several basic functions such as cognition, socialization, and communication. PDDs include autistic disorder, Asperger's disorder, Rett's disorder, childhood disintegrative disorder, and pervasive development disorder not otherwise specified (PDD-NOS) by the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-IV-TR). In the DSM-5 classification, the diagnosis of autistic disorder spectrum disorder encompasses the previous DSM-IV-TR diagnoses of autistic disorder, Asperger's disorder, childhood disintegrative disorder, and PDD-NOS. Autistic disorder is characterized by severe and pervasive impairment of several developmental functions, such as reciprocal social interaction skills, verbal and nonverbal communication, and the presence of stereotyped behavior, interests, and activities. Asperger's syndrome is similar to autistic disorder in that it is characterized by social and verbal impairments but differs in that it shows relatively normal language and cognitive development.

The etiology of PDD is hitherto unknown, but brain dysfunctions have been implicated in children and adults with PDD. Many studies have investigated whether functions of various brain regions are altered in PDD. However, none indicates a unified view on the specific regions that are affected in PDD. Considering that the core symptoms of PDD include impaired communication and social skills and that the frontal lobes are known to participate in these functions, it is likely that frontal lobe dysfunctions play a major role in PDD.

Several neuropsychological studies have revealed impaired executive functions in PDD patients. Executive functions are a set of mental processes that controls goal-directed behavior, which includes planning, working memory, attention, problem solving, verbal reasoning, inhibition, mental flexibility, task switching, and initiation and monitoring of actions. The Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST) is among the most frequently administered neuropsychological tests that assess executive functions. Positron emission tomography (PET) studies have indicated that WCST performance is associated with activity in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) [1]. PDD patients, particularly those with autistic disorder, tend to perform worse on the WCST than healthy subjects by committing a significantly higher number of perseverative errors and achieving fewer categories [2–4].

Recently, altered function of the orbitofrontal cortex (OFC) has been implicated in the neuropsychological pathophysiology of PDD, in the context that PDD patients exhibit impaired social behavior, social interaction, and attention. Bachevalier [5] hypothesized that abnormalities in the orbitofrontal-amygdala circuit may be a fundamental underlying mechanism in PDD. The OFC, especially its right lateral subdivision, appears to play an important role in social cognition [6, 7] and in the pathophysiology of autistic disorder [5, 8, 9]. Performance on the Iowa gambling task (IGT), which evaluates real-life decision-making abilities under ambiguous conditions, is particularly sensitive to changes in OFC function [10, 11]. Nevertheless, only a few studies have used the IGT to examine individuals with PDD.

In the present study, we hypothesized that children and adolescents with high-functioning PDD have lower DLPFC and OFC functions than healthy individuals. To test this hypothesis, we assigned the WCST and IGT to children and adolescents with high-functioning PDD and compared their performance on these tests with that of gender-, age-, and intelligence-matched healthy subjects.

Methods

Subjects

Study participants included 19 Japanese patients with autistic disorder or Asperger's disorder (aged 10–15 years) who were admitted as either inpatients or outpatients at Kohnodai Hospital, National Center for Global Health and Medicine. In order to include only patients with overt clinical features of PDD, those diagnosed as PDD-NOS were not included in the study. Two trained child and adolescent psychiatrists diagnosed their condition according to the DSM-IV-TR criteria [12]. Patients with a current or past diagnosis of mood disorders, schizophrenia, substance-related disorders, attention-deficit and disruptive behavior disorders, or mental retardation were excluded from this study. Eleven patients (ten males and one female) were diagnosed with autistic disorder, and eight patients (seven males and one female) were diagnosed with Asperger's syndrome. Normal healthy controls were recruited from two local public schools by word of mouth, and a total of 47 students volunteered to participate. A child psychiatrist conducted a 30-min clinical interview with each potential control subject to rule out any prevalent or past history of psychiatric disorders. Subjects with prevalent or past history of psychiatric diagnosis, those who underwent treatment for psychiatric or psychological disorders, or those who had a history of absenteeism from school were excluded from the control group. Two of the 47 volunteers were excluded because they exhibited obsessive symptoms: one because of tic disorder, and three because of PDD. After matching these subjects with the patients for gender, age, handedness, and intelligence, 19 volunteers were enrolled as control subjects.

Assessment

Handedness was determined for each subject using the hand usage questionnaire [13]. The IGT [14] was assigned by a trained child and adolescent psychiatrist following the previously described procedures of the original IGT version. The only difference from the original task was that play money was converted from US dollars to Japanese yen [15]. The IGT requires subjects to repeatedly select 100 cards from four decks and maximize their profit. The goal is to win as much money as possible, or, as far as possible, avoid losing money. To achieve this, subjects must discover the most advantageous decks and preferentially select cards from them. Each time they turn over a card, subjects win money; however, when turning over a card, subjects sometimes have to pay a penalty according to a preprogrammed reward/punishment schedule. Gains and losses are different for each card selected from the four decks. Although they yield 10,000 yen rewards, decks A and B are 'disadvantageous’ because their penalties are higher than those of decks C and D. Therefore, they cost more in the long run. These decks are also termed high-paying decks. Decks C and D are 'advantageous’ because while they yield only 5,000 yen, their penalties are lower in amount to gains in the long term. These decks are also termed low-paying decks. Thus, successful task performance requires frequent sampling from decks C and D than from decks A and B. In the present study, subjects were informed that they would get a special gift if they earned at least 300,000 yen at the end of the task [16]. The 100 selections were divided into five blocks of 20 selections each, which allowed us to verify changes in selection patterns throughout the experiment. We recorded the number of disadvantageous cards selected in the overall task and in each block of 20 selections. Four clinical psychologists individually administered the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-Third Edition and the Keio version of the WCST [17]. The WCST was used to determine executive function or abstract reasoning that involves working memory [1] and to examine DLPFC function [18]. In the WCST, we evaluated the number of categories achieved (CA), total errors (TE), and perseverative errors (PE) as reported by Nelson [19]. Categories achieved indicated the ability to change abstract categories, and PEs represent perseveration of preceding errors or the inability to inhibit preceding incorrect responses.

Statistical analyses

Data were analyzed using the Statistical Package for the Social Sciences (SPSS; IBM, Armonk, NY, USA) version 16 for Windows (Microsoft, Redmond, WA, USA). We compared continuous variables (i.e., age and IQ) using t tests and ordinal variables using the Mann-Whitney test. The results were considered statistically significant at p < 0.05.

Ethical considerations

This study was designed in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki and approved by the Ethics Committee of the National Center for Global Health and Medicine. Written informed consent was obtained from all participants and their parents after they had received a description of the study.

Results

Demographic and clinical characteristics of the study participants are summarized in Table 1. PDD patients had not been taking medications at the time of the study. The following comorbidities were observed in the patients: stereotypic movement disorder (three patients), tic disorders (two patients), trichotillomania (one patient), selective mutism (one patient), and specific phobias (one patient). In the WCST, PDD patients achieved significantly fewer categories (CA) and committed significantly more TEs and PEs than the controls (Table 2). During the last selection of the IGT (selections 81–100), the number of disadvantageous card selected was significantly higher in PDD patients than in controls (Figure 1). During the other selections in IGT, we observed no significant differences between PDD patients and the controls.

Figure 1
figure 1

Order of card selection in the Iowa gambling task from the first to the fifth selection. The solid and dotted lines show the mean number of disadvantageous cards drawn by PDD patients and controls, respectively, in each selection of 20 cards. Horizontal bars indicate the standard error of the means. Children with PDD chose more cards from the disadvantageous deck towards the end of the task. *P < 0.05 (Mann-Whitney test, significantly greater than controls).

Table 1 Subject characteristics
Table 2 Subject performance on the Wisconsin card scoring test

Discussion

In the present study, PDD patients committed significantly more perseverative errors and achieved fewer categories in the WCST than the controls. Our results are consistent with those of previous studies in which WCST was assigned to individuals with high-functioning autistic disorder in late adolescence and early adulthood [3, 4]. Patients with DLPFC damage also show more perseverative errors in the WCST than controls [20], and DLPFC activation measured by PET [21] and functional magnetic resonance (fMRI) [22] is increased in subjects performing the WCST. Proton magnetic resonance spectroscopy has revealed similar dysfunction in the left DLPFC and anterior cingulate cortex in children with autistic disorder [23]. Courchesne et al. [24] reported that the mean number and size of DLPFC and medial prefrontal cortex neurons are higher than those of control subjects. Thus, the results of the WCST in the present study are consistent with the findings of previous studies employing anatomical or neuroimaging techniques.

In the present study, PDD patients showed a significantly stronger tendency toward selecting disadvantageous cards in the IGT than the controls, even during the late examination period. To our knowledge, only one other study has investigated the results of IGT assigned to children and adolescents with PDD, which showed no significant difference between patients with Asperger's syndrome and controls [25]. This study showed that the percentage for selecting disadvantageous cards in the final selection in the control group was 31.6%, whereas that of the Asperger's syndrome group was 43.6%. We suspect that the nonsignificant results could be because of a smaller sample size of patients or the low severity of PDD that they exhibited. Future studies with larger sample sizes are needed to elucidate this relationship. Other studies have reported that OFC-injured patients also show a tendency to select disadvantage cards continuously in the later selections [10, 11].

Using morphometric MRI, Hardan et al. [26] observed that the total volume (i.e., gray plus white matter) in the right lateral OFC was decreased in children and adolescents with autistic disorder but was increased in adults with autistic disorder. Moreover, a recent fMRI study of healthy adults reported that the processing theory of mind tasks, a function believed to be impaired in individuals with autistic disorder, was associated with increased activation of the right lateral OFC [7]. The results of the IGT in this study seem to fit with the findings of these previous neuroimaging studies. Thus, considering the results of this study and those of previous studies, PDD patients may have some structural and functional abnormalities in the OFC. Furthermore, the results of this study indicate that both DLPFC and OFC functions are impaired in high-functioning PDD patients in childhood and early adolescence.

The limitations of this study are as follows: (1) the sample size was small, and the PDD subjects were recruited from only one facility; (2) because of a narrow age range and a smaller number of female patients, we could not examine any effects of sex/age differences; (3) only neuropsychological tests were employed in this study. These neuropsychological tests cannot verify frontal lobe dysfunctions directly.

Conclusions

We assigned the WCST and IGT to high-functioning PDD patients in childhood and early adolescence with controls matched for gender, age, handedness, and intelligence. We identified that both DLPFC and OFC functions are impaired in PDD patients. This study is the first to examine the impairment of both DLPFC and OFC functions in children and adolescents with high-functioning PDD. The findings of this study will contribute toward the understanding of brain dysfunction and clinical features of PDD.

To achieve this end, joint studies involving multiple facilities with larger sample sizes and wider age ranges need to be conducted. Moreover, studies evaluating the relationships between frontal lobe functions, and mind tasks and social skills also need to be conducted.

References

  1. Berman KF, Ostrem JL, Randolph C, Gold J, Goldberg TE, Coppola R, Carson RE, Herscovitch P, Weinberger DR: Physiological activation of a cortical network during performance of the Wisconsin card sorting test: a positron emission tomography study. Neuropsychologia. 1995, 33: 1027-1046. 10.1016/0028-3932(95)00035-2.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  2. Liss M, Fein D, Bullard S, Robins D: Brief report: cognitive estimation in individuals with pervasive developmental disorders. J Autism Dev Disord. 2000, 30: 613-618. 10.1023/A:1005699713072.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  3. Rumsey JM: Conceptual problem-solving in highly verbal, nonretarded autistic men. J Autism Dev Disord. 1985, 15: 23-36. 10.1007/BF01837896.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  4. Rumsey JM, Hamburger SD: Neuropsychological divergence of high-level autism and severe dyslexia. J Autism Dev Disord. 1990, 20: 155-168. 10.1007/BF02284715.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. Bachevalier J, Loveland KA: The orbitofrontal-amygdala circuit and self-regulation of social-emotional behavior in autism. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2006, 30: 97-117. 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2005.07.002.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  6. O’Doherty J, Kringelbach ML, Rolls ET, Hornak J, Andrews C: Abstract reward and punishment representations in the human orbitofrontal cortex. Nat Neurosci. 2001, 4: 95-102. 10.1038/82959.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  7. Völlm BA, Taylor ANW, Richardson P, Corcoran R, Stirling J, McKie S, Deakin JFW, Elliott R: Neuronal correlates of theory of mind and empathy: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study in a nonverbal task. Neuroimage. 2006, 29: 90-98. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.07.022.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  8. Dawson G, Munson J, Estes A, Osterling J, McPartland J, Toth K, Carver L, Abbott R: Neurocognitive function and joint attention ability in young children with autism spectrum disorder versus developmental delay. Child Dev. 2002, 73: 345-358. 10.1111/1467-8624.00411.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  9. Salmond CH, De Haan M, Friston KJ, Gadian DG, Vargha-Khadem F: Investigating individual differences in brain abnormalities in autism. Philos Trans R Soc London Ser B Biol Sci. 2003, 358: 405-413. 10.1098/rstb.2002.1210.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  10. Bechara A, Damasio H, Tranel D, Anderson SW: Dissociation of working memory from decision making within the human prefrontal cortex. J Neurosci. 1998, 18: 428-437.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  11. Grant S, Contoreggi C, London ED: Drug abusers show impaired performance in a laboratory test of decision making. Neuropsychologia. 2000, 38: 1180-1187. 10.1016/S0028-3932(99)00158-X.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  12. American Psychological Association: Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. 2000, Washington DC: American Psychological Association, 4

    Google Scholar 

  13. Chapman LJ, Chapman JP: The measurement of handedness. Brain Cognition. 1987, 6: 175-183. 10.1016/0278-2626(87)90118-7.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  14. Bechara A, Damasio AR, Damasio H, Anderson SW: Insensitivity to future consequences following damage to human prefrontal cortex. Cognition. 1984, 50: 7-15.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  15. Fukui H, Murai T, Fukuyama H, Hayashi T, Hanakawa T: Functional activity related to risk anticipation during performance of the Iowa gambling task. Neuroimage. 2005, 24: 253-259. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.08.028.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  16. Kodaira M, Iwadare Y, Ushijima H, Oiji A, Kato M, Sugiyama N, Sasayama D, Usami M, Watanabe K, Saito K: Poor performance on the Iowa gambling task in children with obsessive-compulsive disorder. Ann Gen Psychiatry. 2012, 11: 25-10.1186/1744-859X-11-25.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  17. Igarashi K, Oguni H, Osawa M, Awaya Y, Kato M, Mimura M, Kashima H: Wisconsin card sorting test in children with temporal lobe epilepsy. Brain Dev. 2002, 24: 174-178. 10.1016/S0387-7604(02)00024-4.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  18. Nelson HE: A modified card sorting test sensitive to frontal lobe defects. Cortex. 1976, 12: 313-324. 10.1016/S0010-9452(76)80035-4.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  19. Lezak MD: Neuropsychological Assessment. 1995, Oxford: Oxford University Press

    Google Scholar 

  20. Milner B: Effects of different brain lesions on card sorting: the role of the frontal lobes. Arch Neurol. 1963, 9: 90-100. 10.1001/archneur.1963.00460070100010.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  21. Cabeza R, Nyberg L: Imaging cognition II: an empirical review of 275 PET and fMRI studies. J Cogn Neurosci. 2000, 12: 1-47.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  22. Konishi S, Nakajima K, Uchida I, Kameyama M, Nakahara K, Sekihara K, Miyashita Y: Transient activation of inferior prefrontal cortex during cognitive set shifting. Nat Neurosci. 1998, 1: 80-84. 10.1038/283.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  23. Fujii E, Mori K, Miyazaki M, Hashimoto T, Harada M, Kagami S: Function of the frontal lobe in autistic individuals: a proton magnetic resonance spectroscopic study. JMI. 2010, 57: 35-44.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  24. Courchesne E, Mouton PR, Calhoun ME, Semendeferi K, Ahrens-Barbeau C, Hallet MJ, Barnes CC, Pierce K: Neuron number and size in prefrontal cortex of children with autism. JAMA. 2011, 306: 2001-2010. 10.1001/jama.2011.1638.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  25. Johnson SA, Yechiam E, Murphy RR, Queller S, Stout JC: Motivational processes and autonomic responsivity in Asperger's syndrome: evidence from the Iowa gambling task. JINS. 2006, 12: 668-676.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  26. Hardan AY, Girgis RR, Lacerda ALT, Yorbik O, Kilpatrick M, Keshavan MS, Minshew NJ: Magnetic resonance imaging study of the orbitofrontal cortex in autism. J Child Neurol. 2006, 21: 866-871. 10.1177/08830738060210100701.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

This study was funded by a research grant for Nervous and Mental Disorders (20B-6) of the Japanese Ministry of Health, Labour, and Welfare. We would like to express our deep gratitude to Professor Katsutoshi Tanaka, Professor Hirokuni Tagaya, and Professor Yumi Iwamitsu for giving us good opinions and advice. Moreover, we feel deeply grateful for the children, caregivers, and staff members who participated in this study.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Arata Oiji.

Additional information

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Authors’ contributions

TS wrote the manuscript. MK and AO designed the study. MK, YI, HU, MU, and KW recruited and screened the study participants. MK and YI diagnosed the study participants. MK administered the IGT. TS undertook the statistical analysis. AO and KS supervised the data analysis and manuscript writing. DS gave critical comments on the manuscript. All authors have contributed to and approved the final manuscript.

Authors’ original submitted files for images

Below are the links to the authors’ original submitted files for images.

Authors’ original file for figure 1

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is published under license to BioMed Central Ltd. This is an Open Access article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License ( https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/2.0 ), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Sawa, T., Kodaira, M., Oiji, A. et al. Dysfunction of orbitofrontal and dorsolateral prefrontal cortices in children and adolescents with high-functioning pervasive developmental disorders. Ann Gen Psychiatry 12, 31 (2013). https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1186/1744-859X-12-31

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1186/1744-859X-12-31

Keywords